A man is a
wolf to another man, so Hobbes. Man is selfish and egoistic by nature, he
thinks, and in order to protect men against other men they must be forced to
cooperate. That’s why there are states. States force men to work together and
to help each other, if necessary, so that in the end everybody is better off.
Others, however, think that men are good by nature and that men are by far that
selfish as Hobbes thinks. According to Rousseau, for example, men are helpful
and cooperative and it’s just the state that corrupts them. Which view is
correct? In his first Tanner Lecture on Human Values in 2008 Michael Tomasello tried
to answer this question.
Three main
types of human altruism can be distinguished, so Tomasello: You can share
goods like food with others. You can help others like fetching an out-of-reach
object for someone. Or you can altruistically give information to
another person. What can we say about the presence of these types in man? All
men are more or less sharing, helping and informative but are we naturally so
or are these characteristics forced upon us and do we share, help and inform others
only for egoistic reasons? For answering these questions Tomasello presents
several investigations done with young children and chimpanzees.
He first
discusses helping. In one study (Tomasello pp. 6ff), of the 24 eighteen-month-old
infants tested 22 helped at least once, when a person they didn’t know dropped
something accidentally, but they did nothing, when the person dropped something
on purpose. Tomasello gives several reasons why studies like this one makes it
likely that men help altruistically by nature. First, they show helpful behaviour
already at a very young age before most parents expect their children to behave
pro-socially. Second, parental rewards and encouragement don’t seem to increase
infant’s helpful behaviour. Third, chimpanzees do such things as well. Fourth,
probably such behaviour is shown in different cultures. Fifth, an adult makes a
drawing and another deliberately tears it up. Or, alternatively, an adult puts
aside an empty sheet of paper and another adult tears it up. In the first
situation infants look concerned and want to help more often than in the second
situation. For such reasons, Tomasello believes that “children’s early helping
is not a behavior created by culture and/or parental socialization practices.
Rather, it is an outward expression of children’s natural inclination to sympathize
with others in strife.” (p. 13)
Although
altruistically helping is something both human infants and chimpanzees do in
some situations, altruistically informing others seems typically human.
Take this study, for example: An adult is stapling papers and an infant is
watching it. The adult leaves the room for a moment and another adult comes in
and moves stapler and papers to some shelves. The first adult comes back and
wonders where stapler and papers are. Then most infants will spontaneously point
where they have been put. In another study, a chimpanzee is looking for food.
You see where it is and point to the location. However, the chimp doesn’t
understand, for why should you tell him where to find the food? Any chimpanzee
tries to keep it for himself! Apes don’t point or it should be advantageous for
themselves. Generally they don’t understand what (altruistically) pointing
means, while infants see it as informative behaviour. Such studies show, so Tomasello,
that “the comparison between children and apes is different in the case of
informing. When it comes to informing, as opposed to instrumental helping,
humans do something cooperatively that apes seemingly do not at all. This
suggests that altruism is not a general trait, but rather that altruistic motives
may arise in some domains of activity but not in others.” (pp. 20-21.
And
sharing? “Virtually all experts agree that apes are not very altruistic in the
sharing of resources such as food.” (p. 21) How different children are. Since
Tomasello presents cases of somewhat older children, where learned culture may
have had already an influence, I prefer to give an experience of my own with
the same content. Once on holiday by bike in Norway, my wife and I stopped somewhere.
A man came from a nearby house and began to talk to us in Norwegian and we didn’t
understand a word of it. We were a bit confused and didn’t know what to do.
Then the man left for a moment and returned with a big bag with shrimps and
gave it to us. Why? Till today I have no idea, but I think that it is a case of
altruistically sharing in due form. But back to Tomasello. Do people always
share? Probably not, he says, when your plane crashes in the Andes and you have
one granola bar in your pocket. So, his conclusion is: “In the case of sharing
resources such as food … human children seem to be more generous than
chimpanzees. But … this is only a matter of degree. Starving humans are not so
generous with food, either. It is just that chimpanzees act as if they were
always starving.” (p. 28).
Now I must
cut short my analysis, but anyway, there is little proof that altruism in
helping, informing or sharing is the result of acculturation, parental
intervention, or any other form of socialization. (p. 28) This doesn’t mean
that man is only altruistic by nature, and that helpfulness etc. are not
also promoted by culture in some way. Children learn by themselves, too. They
adapt to what society requires of them, although this adaption, too, is innate to
a large extent. Children follow the rules because they feel to do so and they often
feel ashamed and guilty by themselves if they don’t follow the rules. The
upshot is then: If someone is right, it is not Hobbes but Rousseau. But we
start as a Rousseau and then we become infected by Hobbes.
Source
Michael
Tomasello, Why we cooperate. Cambridge, Mass., etc.: The MIT Press, 2009.
No comments:
Post a Comment